The Study of Orangutans Sheds Light On How Human Curiosity May Have Evolved
Great apes
are curious in captivity, but cautious around unfamiliar objects in the wild,
according to new research.
The study
was published in the journal Scientific Reports, and the researchers claim it
is the first to measure orangutans' responses to novelty in the wild. It
follows the behavior of human-acquired orangutans in a Sumatra reserve.
Great apes
in captivity usually spend a long time exploring new objects. However,
researchers had little idea how that curiosity would play out in the wild.
Previously,
scientists planted novel items throughout the forest at Suaq Balimbing, a
protected biodiversity area in Sumatra where conservation biologists monitor
orangutans. However, in previous experiments, the apes were so wary of the new
items that the experiments failed.
Recently,
biologists devised a more enticing method of pique the orangutans' interest by
planting a piece of tree trunk filled with local forest honey on trees near the
orangutans. The scientists then observed the apes as they encountered the
familiar but intriguingly novel object over the course of 170 trials.
The
biologists discovered that the orangutans' ways of expressing their curiosity
differed depending on their age. Immature orangutans looked at the object
"significantly longer" than other apes, for an average of 233
seconds, compared to 103 seconds for mothers and 34 seconds for sexually mature
males. Immature orangutans were also faster to approach the object and
displayed agitation while doing so.
The apes
rarely touched the branch, instead using a tool like a stick. In areas where
food was plentiful, they were also less likely to approach the honey-stuffed
branch.
However,
their curiosity appeared to be influenced by social connections: when an
orangutan was accompanied by another ape, it was more likely to approach and
touch the object.
According to
a press release, the findings shed light on how human curiosity may have
evolved.
"We
often think of learning and innovation as solo acts, but this might not have
been the case in our early history," said Caroline Schuppli, director of
the Suaq Balimbing orangutan research project and the study's first author.
"If novelty was the spark, then our social lives might have provided the
accelerant."
Comments
Post a Comment